while the hysterical phenomena sometimes associated
with revivals are well known. The supposed prevalence
among prostitutes would not be evidence against the
sexual relationships of hysteria; it has, however,
been denied, even by so great an authority as Parent-Duchatelet
who found it very rare, even in prostitutes in hospitals,
when it was often associated with masturbation; in
prostitutes, however, who returned to a respectable
life, giving up their old habits, he found hysteria
common and severe.[270] The frequent absence of physical
sexual feeling, again, may quite reasonably be taken
as evidence of a disorder of the sexual emotions,
while the undoubted fact that sexual intercourse usually
has little beneficial effect on pronounced hysteria,
and that sexual excitement during sleep and sexual
hallucinations are often painful in the same condition,
is far from showing that injury or repression of the
sexual emotions had nothing to do with the production
of the hysteria. It would be as reasonable to
argue that the evil effect of a heavy meal on a starving
man must be taken as evidence that he was not suffering
from starvation. The fact, indeed, on which Gilles
de la Tourette and others have remarked, that the
hysterical often desire not so much sexual intercourse
as simple affection, would tend to show that there
is here a real analogy, and that starvation or lesion
of the sexual emotions may produce, like bodily starvation,
a rejection of those satisfactions which are demanded
in health. Thus, even a mainly
a priori
examination of the matter may lead us to see that
many arguments brought forward in favor of Charcot’s
position on this point fall to the ground when we realize
that the sexual emotions may constitute a highly complex
sphere, often hidden from observation, sometimes not
conscious at all, and liable to many lesions besides
that due to the non-satisfaction of sexual desire.
At the same time we are not thus enabled to overthrow
any of the positive results attained by Charcot and
his school.
It may, however, be pointed out that Charcot’s
attitude toward hysteria was the outcome of his own
temperament. He was primarily a neurologist,
the bent of his genius was toward the investigation
of facts that could be objectively demonstrated.
His first interest in hysteria, dating from as far
back as 1862, was in hystero-epileptic convulsive attacks,
and to the last he remained indifferent to all facts
which could not be objectively demonstrated.
That was the secret of the advances he was enabled
to make in neurology. For purely psychological
investigation he had no liking, and probably no aptitude.
Anyone who was privileged to observe his methods of
work at the Salpetriere will easily recall the great
master’s towering figure; the disdainful expression,
sometimes, even, it seemed, a little sour; the lofty
bearing which enthusiastic admirers called Napoleonic.
The questions addressed to the patient were cold,
distant, sometimes impatient. Charcot clearly
had little faith in the value of any results so attained.
One may well believe, also, that a man whose superficial
personality was so haughty and awe-inspiring to strangers
would, in any case, have had the greatest difficulty
in penetrating the mysteries of a psychic world so
obscure and elusive as that presented by the hysterical.[271]