Further, the tendency toward the weakening of final
syllables was very strong even then and had been manifesting
itself in one way and another for centuries. I
believe that these further facts help us to understand
the actual sequence of phonetic changes. We may
go so far as to say that the
o (and
u)
could afford to stay the change to
oe (and
ue) until the destructive drift had advanced
to the point where failure to modify the vowel would
soon result in morphological embarrassment. At
a certain moment the _-i_ ending of the plural (and
analogous endings with
i in other formations)
was felt to be too weak to quite bear its functional
burden. The unconscious Anglo-Saxon mind, if
I may be allowed a somewhat summary way of putting
the complex facts, was glad of the opportunity afforded
by certain individual variations, until then automatically
canceled out, to have some share of the burden thrown
on them. These particular variations won through
because they so beautifully allowed the general phonetic
drift to take its course without unsettling the morphological
contours of the language. And the presence of
symbolic variation (
sing,
sang,
sung)
acted as an attracting force on the rise of a new
variation of similar character. All these factors
were equally true of the German vocalic shift.
Owing to the fact that the destructive phonetic drift
was proceeding at a slower rate in German than in
English, the preservative change of
uo to
uee
(
u to
ue) did not need to set in until
300 years or more after the analogous English change.
Nor did it. And this is to my mind a highly significant
fact. Phonetic changes may sometimes be unconsciously
encouraged in order to keep intact the psychological
spaces between words and word forms. The general
drift seizes upon those individual sound variations
that help to preserve the morphological balance or
to lead to the new balance that the language is striving
for.
I would suggest, then, that phonetic change is compacted
of at least three basic strands: (1) A general
drift in one direction, concerning the nature of which
we know almost nothing but which may be suspected to
be of prevailingly dynamic character (tendencies, e.g.,
to greater or less stress, greater or less voicing
of elements); (2) A readjusting tendency which aims
to preserve or restore the fundamental phonetic pattern
of the language; (3) A preservative tendency which
sets in when a too serious morphological unsettlement
is threatened by the main drift. I do not imagine
for a moment that it is always possible to separate
these strands or that this purely schematic statement
does justice to the complex forces that guide the
phonetic drift. The phonetic pattern of a language
is not invariable, but it changes far less readily
than the sounds that compose it. Every phonetic
element that it possesses may change radically and
yet the pattern remain unaffected. It would be
absurd to claim that our present English pattern is
identical with the old Indo-European one, yet it is
impressive to note that even at this late day the
English series of initial consonants: